Parents’ Role in Fostering Young Children’s Learning and Language Development | Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development (2024)

Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda, PhD, Eileen T. Rodriguez, PhD
New York University, USA
, Rev. ed.

PDF version

Introduction

During the first years of life, children undergo major developmental changes across a range of domains. In particular, the entry into “formal language” is one of the most heralded achievements of early development. Language enables children to share meanings with others, and to participate in cultural learning in unprecedented ways. Moreover, language is foundational to children’s school readiness and achievement. For these reasons, a vast body of research has been dedicated to understanding the social-contextual factors that support children’s early language and learning. This work is also central to practitioners, educators and policy makers who seek to promote positive developmental outcomes in young children.

Subject

Developmental scholars have long been interested in documenting the social experiences that help explain within- and between-group variation in children’s early language and learning.1,2 This work is anchored in the writings of scholars such as Bruner3,4 and Vygotsky,5 who posited that learning occurs in a socio-cultural context in which adults and primary caregivers support or “scaffold” young children to higher levels of thinking and acting. According to this view, children who experience sensitive, cognitively stimulating home environments early in development are at an advantage in the learning process.

Problem

Research into the factors that promote positive language growth and learning in young children is central to addressing achievement gaps that exist in children from different ethnic, language, racial, and socioeconomic backgrounds. Children enter school with different levels of skill, and these initial differences often affect children’s subsequent language growth, cognitive development, literacy and academic achievement.6,7,8 Children who exhibit delays at the onset of schooling are at risk for early academic difficulties and are also more likely to experience grade retention, special education placement, and failure to complete high school.9,10,11

These delays are particularly evident in children living in poverty. Children from low-income households lag behind their peers in language skills from early on,2,12 and have been shown to develop vocabularies at slower rates than their peers from more economically advantaged households.7 Smaller receptive and productive vocabularies, in turn, predict children’s later reading and spelling difficulties in school.8,13

Research Context

The demographic profiles of minority and immigrant populations in the U.S. and Canada have changed dramatically over the past decade ̶ a shift that has generated research on the widespread disparities that exist in children’s school readiness across ethnic, racial and socioeconomic lines.14,15,16,17,18 Because group disparities in learning exist prior to kindergarten, researchers and practitioners alike seek to understand the role of children’s early home environment in the learning process.19,20,21,22,23

Research Questions

Inquiry into the role of the home environment on young children’s language and learning can be classified under two broad questions:

  1. Which aspects of parenting matter for children’s early language and learning, and why?
  2. What factors enable parents to provide a supportive environment to their young children?

Recent Research Results

Which aspects of parenting matter, and why?

Three aspects of parenting have been highlighted as central to children’s early language and learning: (1) the frequency of children’s participation in routine learning activities (e.g., shared bookreading, storytelling); (2) the quality of caregiver-child engagements (e.g., parents’ cognitive stimulation and sensitivity/responsiveness); and (3) the provision of age-appropriate learning materials (e.g., books and toys).24

Early and consistent participation in routine learning activities, such as shared book reading, storytelling, and teaching about the letters of the alphabet, provide children with a critical foundation for early learning, language growth and emergent literacy.25,26,27,28Routine activities provide young children with a familiar structure for interpreting others’ behaviors and language, anticipating the temporal sequencing of events, and drawing inferences from new experiences.29,30 Moreover, engagement in learning activities expands children’s vocabularies and conceptual knowledge.31 In particular, shared bookreading, as well as the sharing of oral stories, facilitate young children’s vocabulary growth, phonemic skills, print concept knowledge, and positive attitudes toward literacy.25,27,32,33,34,35

A plethora of studies also indicate that the quality of parent-caregiver interactions plays a formative role in children’s early language and learning. In fact, the amount and style of language that parents use when conversing with their children is one of the strongest predictors of children’s early language. Children benefit from exposure to adult speech that is varied and rich in information about objects and events in the environment.7,36,37 Additionally, parents who contingently respond to their young children’s verbal and exploratory initiatives (through verbal descriptions and questions) tend to have children with more advanced receptive and productive language, phonological awareness, and story comprehension skills.38,39,40,41

Finally, the provision of learning materials (e.g., books, toys that facilitate learning) has been shown to support young children’s language growth and learning.42,43,44 Learning materials provide opportunities for caregiver-child exchanges about specific objects and actions, such as when a parent and child pretend to cook a meal. In such instances, materials serve as a vehicle for communicative exchanges around a shared topic of conversation. Specifically, exposure to toys that enable symbolic play and support the development of fine motor skills has been shown to relate to children’s early receptive language skills, intrinsic motivation and positive approaches to learning.45,46 In addition, children’s familiarity with storybooks has been linked to their receptive and expressive vocabularies and early reading abilities.26,27

What factors predict positive parenting?

Researchers agree that parenting is multiply determined by characteristics of both parents and children. In terms of parent characteristics, parent age, education, income, and race/ethnicity (to name a few) have all been shown to relate to the three aspects of parenting discussed above. For example, compared to older mothers, teen mothers display lower levels of verbal stimulation and involvement, higher levels of intrusiveness, and maternal speech that is less varied and complex.47,48 Mothers with fewer years of education read to their children less frequently25,49 and demonstrate less sophisticated language and literacy skills themselves,50 which affects the quantity and quality of their verbal interactions with their children.2 Parental education, in turn, relates to household income: poverty and persistent poverty are strongly associated with less stimulating home environments,51 and parents living in poverty have children who are at risk for cognitive, academic, and social-emotional difficulties.52,53 Finally, Hispanic and African American mothers are, on average, less likely to read to their children than White, non-Hispanic mothers;54 and Spanish-speaking Hispanic families have fewer children’s books available in the home as compared to their non-Hispanic counterparts.25 These racial and ethnic findings are likely explained by differences in family resources across groups, as minority status is often associated with various social demographic risks.

Child characteristics, such as gender and birth order (as two of many examples), have also been linked to early measures of language and learning. For example, girls tend to have a slight advantage over boys in the early stages of vocabulary development,55,56,57 and studies have documented that families spend substantially more time in literacy-related activities with girls than with boys.58 Firstborn children have slightly larger vocabularies on average than their later-born peers.59 Further, mothers differ in their language, engagement and responsiveness toward their first- and laterborn children, with input favoring firstborns.60

Research Gaps

In light of evidence that children from low-income and minority backgrounds are more likely to exhibit delays in language and learning at school entry, additional work is needed to understand why these differences exist, and how to best support parents in their provision of positive home environments for their children. Future research should investigate the ways in which multiple aspects of the home learning environment jointly contribute to developmental outcomes in children. Moreover, studies on “school readiness” should begin at the earliest stages of infancy, as this is the period when foundational language and knowledge develops. In this regard, research on the language development and school readiness of children from language minority households should focus on how in- and out-of-home language experiences jointly contribute to children’s proficiency in both English and their native language. Finally, most research on the social context of children’s language and learning is focused on children’s interactions with mothers. Given the rich social networks that comprise infants’ and toddlers’ environments, future studies should examine the literacy opportunities offered by multiple members of young children’s social worlds, including fathers, siblings, extended family members, and childcare providers.

Conclusions

There exists irrefutable evidence for the importance of children’s early language and learning for later school readiness, engagement and performance. Children’s experiences at home are critical to early language growth and learning. In particular, three aspects of the home literacy environment promote children’s learning and language: learning activities (e.g., daily book reading), parenting quality (e.g., responsiveness), and learning materials (e.g., age-appropriate toys and books). Additionally, parents with more resources (e.g., education, income) are better able to provide positive learning experiences for their young children. Finally, children also play a key role in their own learning experiences, as exemplified by links between child characteristics and parenting behaviors. Children affect parents just as parents affect children; it is therefore critical to acknowledge the transactional nature of children’s early language and learning experiences.61

Implications

Research on children’s early learning environments is relevant to policy makers, educators, and practitioners who seek to promote the positive language development and learning of young children. Intervention and preventive efforts should target multiple aspects of children’s early language and learning environments, including supporting parents in their provision of literacy-promoting activities, sensitive and responsive engagements, and age-appropriate materials that facilitate learning. Moreover, these efforts should begin early in development, as children are likely to benefit most from supportive home environments during the formative years of rapid language growth and learning.22,62,63 Finally, interventions with parents that aim to support children’s learning should attend to the cultural context of early development when working with parents from different backgrounds, and also consider the broader social context of parenting by attending to the barriers created by poverty and low parental education.

References

  1. Burns MS, Griffin P, Snow CE, eds. Starting out Right: A Guide to Promoting children’s Reading Succes. Specific Recommendations from America's Leading Researchers on How To Help Children Become Successful Readers . Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1999.
  2. Hoff E. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 2003;74(3):1368-1378.
  3. Bruner JS. The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 1975;2(1): 1-19.
  4. Bruner J. Child’s Talk: Learning to use Language . New York, NY: Norton; 1983
  5. Vygotsky LS. Myshlenie i rech’. [Thought and language]. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1962.
  6. Dickinson DK. Bridges to Literacy: Children, Families, and Schools . Cambridge, MA: Blackwell; 1994.
  7. Hart B, Risley T. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experiences of Young American Children . Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes; 1995.
  8. Snow CE, Porche MV, Patton ED, Tabors PO, Harris SR. Is Literacy Enough? Pathways to Academic Success for Adolescents . Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing; 2007.
  9. Campbell FA, Ramey CT. Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families. Child Development 1994;65(2):684-698.
  10. Kaufman P, Alt MN, Chapman CD. Dropout Rates in the United States: 2001. Statistical Analysis Report NCES 2005-046 . Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics; 2004.
  11. Lee VE, Burkam DT. Inequity at the Starting Gate: Social Background differences in Achievement as Children Begin School . Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute, 2002.
  12. Nord CW, Lennon J, Liu B, Chandler K. Home literacy activities and signs of children’s emerging literacy: 1993 and 1999 . Education Statistics Quarterly 2000;2(1):19-27.
  13. Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, Pagani LS, Feinstein L, Engel M, Brook-Guns J, Sexton H, Duckworth K, Japel C. School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology 2007;43(3):1428-1446.
  14. Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics. America’s Children: Key National Indicators of Well-Being, 2005. Washington, DC: Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics; 2009.
  15. Ferguson HB, Bovaird S, Mueller MP. The impact of poverty on educational outcomes for children. Paediatrics & Child Health 2007;12(8):701–706.
  16. KewalRamani A, Gilbertson L, Fox M, Provasnik S. Status and Trends in the Education of Racial and Ethnic Minorities (NCES 2007-039). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences. U.S. Department of Education; 2007.
  17. Rouse CE, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan S, eds. School readiness: Closing racial and ethnic gaps. The Future of Children 2005;15(1):1-195.
  18. Thomas EM. Readiness to Learn at School among five-year-old Children in Canada.Catalogue No. 89-599-MIE. No. 004. Ottawa, ON: Statistics Canada.
  19. Beals DE, DeTemple JM, Dickinson DK. Talking and listening that support early literacy development of children from low-income families. In: Dickinson DK, ed. Bridges to literacy: Children, Families, and Schools . Cambridge, MA: Blackwell; 1994:19-40.
  20. Chall JS, Jacobs VA, Baldwin LE. The Reading Crisis: Why Poor Children Fall Behind . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1990.
  21. Dickinson DK, DeTemple J. Putting parents in the picture: Maternal reports of preschoolers’ literacy as a predictor of early reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 1998;13(2):241-261
  22. Shonkoff J, Phillips D. From Neurons to Neighborhoods. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.
  23. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders to readers. In: Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, eds. Handbook of Early Literacy Research . New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2003:11-29.
  24. Rodriguez ET, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Spellmann ME, Pan BA, Raikes H, Lugo-Gil J, Luze G. The formative role of home literacy experiences across the first three years of life in children from low-income families. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology . In press.
  25. Raikes H, Pan BA, Luze G, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Brooks-Gunn J, Constantine J, Tarullo LB, Raikes HA, Rodriguez ET. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development 2006;77(4):924-953.
  26. Payne AC, Whitehurst GJ, Angell AL. The role of home literacy environment in the development of language ability in preschool children from low-income families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 1994;9(3-4):427-440.
  27. Senechal M, LeFevre JA, Hudson E, Lawson P. Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children’s vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology 1996;88(3):520‑536.
  28. Snow CE, Dickinson DK. Social sources of narrative skills at home and at school. First Language 1990;10(29):87-103.
  29. Nelson K. How children represent knowledge of their world in and out of language. In: Siegler RS, ed. Children’s Thinking: What Develops? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978:255-273.
  30. Nelson K. Event Knowledge: Structure and Function in Development . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1986.
  31. Bloom L. Language acquisition in its developmental context. In: Damon W, ed. Handbook of Child Psychology. 5th ed. New York, NY: J. Wiley; 1998:309-370. Kuhn D, Siegler RS, eds. Cognition, perception, and language; vol 2.
  32. DeBaryshe BD. Joint picture-book reading correlates of early oral language skill. Journal of Child Language 1993;20(2):455-462.
  33. Dickinson DK, Tabors PO. Early literacy: Linkages between home, school and literacy achievement at age five. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 1991;6(1):30-46
  34. Lyytinen P, Laasko M, Poikkeus A. Parental contributions to child’s early language and interest in books. European Journal of Psychology of Education 1998;13(3):297-308.
  35. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology 1994;30(1):73-87.
  36. Evans GW, Maxwell LE, Hart B. Parental language and verbal responsiveness to children in crowded homes. Developmental Psychology 1999;35(4):1020-1023.
  37. Weizman ZO, Snow CE. Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology 2001;37(2):265-279.
  38. Beals DE, DeTemple JM. Home contributions to early language and literacy development. National Reading Conference Yearbook 1993;42:207-215.
  39. Hann DM, Osofsky JD, Culp AM. Relating the adolescent mother-child relationship to preschool outcomes. Infant Mental Health Journal 1996;17(4): 302-209.
  40. Silven M, Niemi P, Voeten M. Do maternal interaction and early language predict phonological awareness in 3-to-4-year olds? Cognitive Development 2002;17(1): 1133-1155.
  41. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L. Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development 2001;72(3): 748-767.
  42. Neuman SB, Roskos K. Access to print for children of poverty: Differential effects of adult mediation and literacy-enriched play settings on environmental and functional print tasks. American Educational Research Journal 1993;30(1): 95-122.
  43. Senechal M, LeFevre JA, Thomas E, Daley K. Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quaterly 1998;33(1):96-116.
  44. Tabors PO, Roach KA, Snow CE. Home language and literacy environment: Final results. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School . Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes;2001:111-138.
  45. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Tamis-LeMonda C, Flynn V, Rovira I, Tineo W, Mendelsohn AL. Books, toys, parent-child interaction, and development in young Latino children. Ambulatory Pediatrics 2006;6(2):72-78.
  46. Gottfried AE, Fleming JS, Gottfried AW. Role of cognitively stimulating home environment in children’s academic intrinsic motivation: A longitudinal study. Child Development 1998;69(5):1448-1460.
  47. Keown LJ, Woodward LJ, Field J. Language development of pre-school children born to teenage mothers. Infant and Child Development 2001;10(3):129-145.
  48. Whiteside-Mansell L, Pope SK, Bradley RH. Patterns of parenting behavior in young mothers. Family Relations 1996;45(3):273-281.
  49. Scarborough HS, Dobrich W. On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review 1994;14(3):245-302.
  50. Rowe ML, Pan BA, Ayoub C. Predictors of variation in maternal talk to children: A longitudinal study of low-income families. Parenting: Science and Practice 2005;5(3);259-283.
  51. Garrett P, Ng’andu N, Ferron J. Poverty experiences of young children and the quality of their home environments. Child Development 1994;65(2):331-345.
  52. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children 1997;7(2):55-71.
  53. Smith JR, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. The consequences of living in poverty for young children’s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. In: Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, eds. Consequences of Growing Up Poor.New York: Russell Sage; 1997: 132-189.
  54. Yarosz DJ, Barnett WS. Who reads to young children? Identifying predictors of reading of family reading activities. Reading Psychology 2001;22(1):67-81.
  55. Bornstein MH, Haynes OM, Painter KM. Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language 1998;25(2):367-393.
  56. Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal DJ, Pethick SJ. Variability in Early Communicative Development . Chicago, IL: Chigago Press; 1994. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development; vol 5(59).
  57. Pan BA, Rowe ML, Singer J, Snow CE. Maternal correlates of toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 2005;76(4):763−782.
  58. Teale WH. Home background and young children’s literacy development. In: Teale WH, Sulzby E, eds. Emergent literacy: Writing and reading . Norwood, NJ: Ablex.;1986:173-206.
  59. Hoff-Ginsberg E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics 1998;19(4):603‑631.
  60. Bornstein MH. Parenting Infants. In: Bornstein MH, ed. Handbook of Parenting. 2nd ed., Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates; 2002:3-44. Children and parenting; vol 1.
  61. Sameroff AJ, Fiese BH. Models of development and developmental risk. In: Zeanah CH Jr., ed. Handbook of Infant Mental Health. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2005:3-19.
  62. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Cristofaro TN, Rodriguez ET, Bornstein MH. Early language development: Social influences in the first years of life . In: Balter L, Tamis-LeMonda CS, eds. Child Psychology: A Handbook of Contemporary Issues. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 2006:79-108.
  63. Hirsh-Pasek K, Burchinal M. Mother and caregiver sensitivity over time: Predicting language and academic outcomes with variable- and person-centered approaches. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 2006;52(3):449-485.

Note:

This paper was financed by the Canadian Council on Learning - Early Childhood Learning Knowledge Centre

How to cite this article:

Tamis-LeMonda CS, Rodriguez ET. Parents’ Role in Fostering Young Children’s Learning and Language Development. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Rvachew S, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online].https://www.child-encyclopedia.com/language-development-and-literacy/according-experts/parents-role-fostering-young-childrens-learning. Updated: November 2009. Accessed March 22, 2024.

Text copied to the clipboard ✓

Parents’ Role in Fostering Young Children’s Learning and Language Development | Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development (2024)

FAQs

What is the role of parents in their children's language development? ›

By creating a language-rich environment, being responsive communicators, reading aloud, expanding vocabulary, engaging in conversations, limiting screen time, and seeking professional guidance when necessary, parents can accelerate their children's language development and set them on a path to effective communication ...

How can parents help to develop their children's language skills? ›

You can support language development by talking with your child and responding when your child communicates. Reading books and sharing stories is good for language development. If you're worried about your child's language development, talk with a health professional.

What is the role of the parent in early childhood development? ›

Parents' knowledge of how to meet their children's basic physical (e.g., hunger) and emotional (e.g., wanting to be held or soothed) needs, as well as of how to read infants' cues and signals, can improve the synchronicity between parent and child, ensuring proper child growth and development.

How would a parent or caregiver support language learning in early childhood? ›

Caregivers can try to make reading with children part of their everyday routine. How you read can help improve the child's ability to learn new words. Describe pictures, give a definition for new words, ask questions, and incorporate music. Stories provide an opportunity to make links with your child's experiences.

How parents can support speech and language development? ›

Respond when your baby laughs or makes faces. Make the same faces back to them. Teach your baby to do what you do, like clapping your hands and playing peek-a-boo. Talk to your baby as you give them a bath, feed them, and get them dressed.

What role do parents play in early language intervention? ›

There are many ways that you can play an active role in your child's intervention, such as: helping set goals for your child with the speech language pathologist. describing your daily activities and routines. learning strategies that you can use at home that will build your child's communication skills.

How can parents be involved in their toddlers language development? ›

Acknowledge toddlers' use of language by repeating the words and phrases they say. Also help them learn how to build on language they know by expanding their words into phrases and sentences. If a toddler says, “Mommy go?” You can say, “Yes, mommy is going to work now.”

How can you involve families in your efforts to build early language development? ›

Explain reading practices and invite parents to observe reading instruction by another parent or teacher in person or by providing informal videos. Send home books that are translated to create dual language books (Rowe & Fain, 2013). Include bilingual staff or use translators in family involvement activities.

How can parents and teachers support language development? ›

Children's language skills are enriched and expanded when adults engage them in back-and-forth conversations that extend over multiple turns and encourage the child to play an active role. Asking children to tell personal narratives about their experiences is a good way to initiate these responsive conversations.

What role do parents play in children's learning? ›

In early childhood, parental interactions such as reading aloud, playing, and exploring play a key role in children's development of language, critical reasoning, and social skills. By the way, games also provide an immersive and interactive learning experience that can complement traditional educational methods.

How parents can help early childhood development? ›

Adults can facilitate the development of a child's executive function skills by establishing routines, modeling appropriate social behavior, and creating and maintaining supportive, reliable relationships.

Why is the role of a parent so important to a child's development? ›

Parents play a significant role in helping children build and refine their knowledge and skills, as well as their learning expectations, beliefs, goals, and coping strategies.

How can parents affect their child's language development? ›

In fact, the amount and style of language that parents use when conversing with their children is one of the strongest predictors of children's early language. Children benefit from exposure to adult speech that is varied and rich in information about objects and events in the environment.

What can parents do to encourage an infant's language development? ›

Use actions, sing, make noises and funny faces. Don't be shy, being a bit silly helps get their attention and makes them laugh and can encourage language development.

What is an example of strategies used by parents to support their child's language development? ›

Talk about everyday experiences, show your child pictures, and tell her stories. If you use a different language at home, speak and read to your child in that language. This can help grow his vocabulary and make connections at school. It can also increase his curiosity and readiness to learn at school.

What role does a child's family play in their learning of language? ›

Recent research has demonstrated that mothers simplify their language in several ways when speaking to young children. As the childrens' language increases in complexity, so does the mothers'.

How do parental behaviors affect a child's language development? ›

A recent metanalytic study that examines parental behavior and early childhood language in typically developing samples indicates that children whose caregivers show higher levels of sensitivity and warmth display stronger language skills than children who receive lower levels of such parenting behavior [25].

How are parents important in developing language arts skills? ›

From the moment babies are born, they start developing literacy skills through their relationship with their parents. By talking, reading, singing, and playing with your infant or toddler, you provide the foundation your child will need to develop language and reading skills.

Top Articles
Is Innsbruck Expensive to Visit? | Budget Your Trip
The History Of Paintball | Paintball Park
AllHere, praised for creating LAUSD’s $6M AI chatbot, files for bankruptcy
Danielle Moodie-Mills Net Worth
Maria Dolores Franziska Kolowrat Krakowská
Ret Paladin Phase 2 Bis Wotlk
Prosper TX Visitors Guide - Dallas Fort Worth Guide
Culver's Flavor Of The Day Wilson Nc
Rubfinder
Katie Boyle Dancer Biography
Bernie Platt, former Cherry Hill mayor and funeral home magnate, has died at 90
Culver's Flavor Of The Day Monroe
Es.cvs.com/Otchs/Devoted
Capitulo 2B Answers Page 40
Shooting Games Multiplayer Unblocked
‘Accused: Guilty Or Innocent?’: A&E Delivering Up-Close Look At Lives Of Those Accused Of Brutal Crimes
Bestellung Ahrefs
Dutch Bros San Angelo Tx
Telegram Scat
Dallas Cowboys On Sirius Xm Radio
How Much Is Tay Ks Bail
Little Caesars 92Nd And Pecos
Orange Pill 44 291
Diakimeko Leaks
Aerocareusa Hmebillpay Com
Dulce
Terry Bradshaw | Biography, Stats, & Facts
Two Babies One Fox Full Comic Pdf
Anotherdeadfairy
Parkeren Emmen | Reserveren vanaf €9,25 per dag | Q-Park
Arrest Gif
Darktide Terrifying Barrage
Halsted Bus Tracker
Utexas Baseball Schedule 2023
Kaiju Paradise Crafting Recipes
Upstate Ny Craigslist Pets
2016 Honda Accord Belt Diagram
Senior Houses For Sale Near Me
Why Holly Gibney Is One of TV's Best Protagonists
SF bay area cars & trucks "chevrolet 50" - craigslist
How to play Yahoo Fantasy Football | Yahoo Help - SLN24152
Shane Gillis’s Fall and Rise
2023 Fantasy Football Draft Guide: Rankings, cheat sheets and analysis
At Home Hourly Pay
Tfn Powerschool
Online-Reservierungen - Booqable Vermietungssoftware
Chr Pop Pulse
Egg Inc Wiki
1990 cold case: Who killed Cheryl Henry and Andy Atkinson on Lovers Lane in west Houston?
Dietary Extras Given Crossword Clue
Spn 3464 Engine Throttle Actuator 1 Control Command
The Love Life Of Kelsey Asbille: A Comprehensive Guide To Her Relationships
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Trent Wehner

Last Updated:

Views: 6061

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (56 voted)

Reviews: 87% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Trent Wehner

Birthday: 1993-03-14

Address: 872 Kevin Squares, New Codyville, AK 01785-0416

Phone: +18698800304764

Job: Senior Farming Developer

Hobby: Paintball, Calligraphy, Hunting, Flying disc, Lapidary, Rafting, Inline skating

Introduction: My name is Trent Wehner, I am a talented, brainy, zealous, light, funny, gleaming, attractive person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.